Some
initial findings regarding the theme – Emotion and attitude
Having
spent the last two months analysing and gradually synthesising the findings
from the observation data I’m beginning to make some progress. Below is
an very early overview of
findings relating to the theme ‘emotion and attitude’.
Method
I coded and analysed ‘expressions of affect’ which include
external expressions such as emotionally expressive words, interjections and
exclamations, eye contact or lack thereof, facial expressions and multiple
modes of body-language. Interestingly, the majority of emotive speech
happened privately between two learners or as self-talk. Without the use
of multiple microphones the entire range of emotional expressions would have
been 'under-cover' of the layer of audible classroom dialogue.
The
expressions can be broadly organised by the responses at consistent routines of
the each lesson. Most lessons I observed were structured in three
parts. These were: the initial introduction and demonstration of the
skill/method/concept to be learned; learner application/practice - usually in
groups; review, discussion and evaluation and marking of learner work.
Findings
Anticipation
of failure
Learners reacted strongly to being compelled to participate
in numeracy lessons or activities. I’m struggling with the word
‘compelled’ but given the negative emotion expressed most adults would simply
leave if they could. There are reasons why these learners don’t which
have to do with PTE attendance procedures/rules and sociocultural pressures.
When tutors introduced the topic learners become visibly anxious,
exclaimed their hatred of maths or their poor historical performance with maths
and often begin to withdraw their participation. I’ve linked these
responses to the concept ‘anticipation of failure’. The anticipation of
failure has links to learners’ prior experiences. Learners anticipate
re-experiencing the worst aspects of their mathematics experiences. This
answers one question: do adult numeracy environments activate mathematical
beliefs? Without a doubt. I have been playing with the term
‘emotional inertia’ to describe how difficult it is for a tutor to positively
engage learners in numeracy.
Emotional investment
The second part of the lessons (Learner application – group
problem solving) evokes emotional responses also. But here they are a mix
of positive and negative. On the positive side many learners emotionally
invest into solving tasks, they work hard, persist and care deeply about the
results. They appear motivated by the desire to demonstrate to others and
themselves how good they are. They want to be the best and as such are
performance oriented. These are the learners that challenge tutors, they
are usually the best in the class and interact the most with the tutor during
numeracy sessions. They care deeply about being right and will work hard
to be so.
Learners
also express negative emotions during problem solving phases of lessons.
Often they become overwhelmed by the complexity of the task and
disengage. These learners often are the hardest to engage as they
withdraw at the first sign of difficulty. These learners have deep doubts
about their ability and are motivated to avoid losing status. If the task looks
like it could possibly damage their image they choose not engage. Other
learners do engage and become frustrated at the lack of progress (they often
resemble the positive emotion described above) but these learners are motivated
by the desire to get the job done. They want to finish the task as
quickly as possible. When it proves to be a challenge and they feel they
are no longer moving toward completing the task they begin to get frustrated in
a negative way.
Loading meaning onto correct/incorrect answers
At the third phase of the lesson (during which the tutor
works through the answers) all learners who have completed the tasks are
fixated on the tutor and answers. Many learners describe themselves as
either ‘math people’ or 'non-math people’. Many adult learners use
numeracy classes to either verify their existing identity or to reconstruct
it. Hence many learners ‘load meaning’ onto their answers. When
they have an answer it is not just a quantitative answer to a quantitative
question rather it represents whether one is ‘smart’, ‘intelligent’, ‘good at
maths’ or ‘bad at maths’. There were occasions in the observations where
learners who got answers wrong had huge emotional responses and withdrew
participation from that point on. For these learner’s getting the answer
wrong confirmed their historical recollections and beliefs that they are not
intelligent.
In summary, participating in numeracy is highly emotional
for almost all learners whether they are ‘maths people’ or 'non-maths
people’. Learners load meaning onto what happens in the class, how they
learn, the answers they get correct, and the answers they get incorrect.
Adult learners care about their performance with maths and numeracy. This
is a huge opportunity for educationalists.
The question next is: Why do learners care? What do they believe about numeracy and mathematics that makes it so meaningful for them?
There is a social perception that if we 'get' maths we are smart and if we don't we are not. Therefore in the group this can be revealed and potential for embarrassment and humiliation reigns. The anxiety rate increases not in the ability to achieve the maths but in the ability to save face amongst ones peers!
ReplyDeleteIt's such a shame! I had a student the other day who was really into the lesson. I asked him what he thought the answer was and he almost had a go at it. Then he looked at the others and decided not to say anything. I could see the whole thing playing out in that moment "What if I'm wrong, the others will know I'm no good". Oh man. So close.
ReplyDelete